DOES LATE DIAGNOSIS OF AUTISM AFFECT SELF-ESTEEM?

1 Sep

SELF-ESTEEM WHEN AUTISM IS DIAGNOSED IN ADULTHOOD

By Lirio S. Covey, Ph.D.

Autism is described formally as a lhife-long disorder. Early understanding and knowledge about autism focused on the condition as seen in children but not so much on autism as experienced by adults. Recent years have seen a growing recognition that autism in adults occurs as well. In fact, because there aRe many more autistic persons older than 18 years of age than younger, it follows that there are more adults than children and adolescents who are autistic.

During the past several months, there have been notably more inquiries than in the past received by AAAP regarding autism diagnosed in adulthood. A recent article published in the journal, “Frontiers in Psychology ”, relates to that recent increased attention.

The study was conducted in the United Kingdom, participants were persons who had previously received an autism diagnosis only during their adult years who responded to an online survey. This convenience sample consisted of 151 persons; most were female (77.6%) and more than 50% had attended college or better were White British. The main questions asked in relation to the autism diagnosis obtained as adults concerned their self-esteem, mental wellbeing, and the concept of autism pride. Autism pride was conceived as reflecting pride in autism being a part of oneself and its converse, feelings of dissatisfaction with being autistic.

The researchers found that, among the sample of adult autistics, diagnostic timing (years since the diagnosis) was not correlated with mental wellbeing but was correlated with self-esteem and autism pride. That is, the longer the time from being diagnosed as autistic, the better the self-esteem; on the other hand, lesser years since the time of diagnosis was correlated with greater dissatisfaction with being autistic.

Qualitative research from 54 of the original 151 participants were asked the question of how knowing the autism diagnosis had impacted their thoughts and feelings about themselves. What became apparent was that post-diagnosis, a dynamic process of attitudinal adjustment occurred. Characteristically, the process began with an emotional rejection of the diagnosis brought on by awareness of the stigma associated with autism and frustration that it had taken long to recognize the autism, followed by unfolding self-knowledge and, in time, a new appreciation of their differences from others, and eventual self-acceptance. Contact and support from others during this process exerted beneficial effects on developing self-esteem.

In other words, that the length of time from the autism diagnosis was a predictor of self-esteem indicates that a learning process takes place over time wherein upon learning the diagnosis, the newly diagnosed autistic person begins a self-exploration, understands how and why they are different from others, and can come to a growing appreciation of their positive autistic qualities, even if once presumed by themselves or others as deficits.

Notable limitations of the study that prevent generalization of the findings to other autistics receiving a late diagnosis are: 1) the information was obtained from a convenience and specialized sample – mostly female, well-educated, and White British, and 2) because the data obtained referred to only one time point in the participants’ life, only a presumptive rather than a causal effect of diagnostic timing on self-esteem and autism pride can be inferred.

What also remains to be known is whether, had the autism not been diagnosed earlier or at all, would the ensuing years have also brought on the increase in self-esteem, that is, as a natural effect of the person’s maturation, that is without the autism-related response. What is also of interest, since delayed diagnosis among autistic persons is not uncommon, is that, as suggested by the study findings, obtaining the autism diagnosis later in life can increase rather than decrease self-esteem.

Reference: Kirsten Corden, Rebecca Brewer, Eilidh Cage. “Personal Identity After an Autism Diagnosis: Relationships with Self-Esteem, Mental Wellbeing, and Diagnostic Timing. Frontier in Psychology, July, 2021.

Do autistic persons show what they mean?

1 Sep

How facially expressive are persons with autism?

By Lirio S. Covey, Ph.D.

Facial expression is an important means of communication. Being able to accurately express inner emotion is critical in conducting meaningful social interactions.

A recent article that compiled and analyzed findings from 39 well conducted research studies found that persons with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) are less expressive overall than persons without ASD.

In addition, their facial expressions were found to be less consistent or appropriate to the social context. This characteristic, the authors of the study suggest, likely contributes to the deficits of persons with autism in effecting reciprocal social interactions.

Difficulty in social interaction and communication is one of the two core symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder (Repetitive, compulsive behaviors and resistance to change is the other).

Variations in the extent and degree of this deficit among different autistic persons were noted in the research. When compared with non-ASD persons these differences were smaller with older age and higher intellectual functioning. An implication of this finding is that more knowledge and familiarity with social practices and norms could improve the deficit in appropriate and meaningful facial expression. Such familiarity could come with greater socialization and experiences in variable social contexts and experiences in wider social settings.

This would imply that greater socialization and experiences with persons without ASD, an outcome of inclusion practices in employment, recreational, and educational settings, could ameliorate, over time, the lack of social competencies notable in persons with ASD.

Reference: Facial Expression production in Autism: a Meta-analysis. Dominic A. Trevisan

Maureen Hoskyn, Elina Birminghamm Autism Research, December 2018

SYNESTHESIA

12 Oct

SYNESTHESIA, AUTISM, SAVANT SYNDROME.

Is there a relationship?

By Lirio S. Covey, Ph.D.

Do you feel sounds and taste shapes? Do you smell songs or hear flavors?

No, you may not be hallucinating; you could be truly experiencing this phenomenon of simultaneously felt multiple experiences. You could be a synesthete, someone with synesthesia!

Synesthesia is a neurological condition wherein perception of one sensory modality produces simultaneous awareness of one or more other sensory modalities. For example, hearing music can stimulate a sense of sweetness or the feel of a tickle on a body part. Or when reading letters of the alphabet, each letter is viewed in color, varying from one letter to another. Or hearing a certain word triggers the taste of a favored drink.

Why is this of interest to autism?

The estimated prevalence of synesthesia in the general population is 2-4%; among autistic persons, the estimated prevalence of synesthesia is much higher – about 20%. Considered distinct conditions, yet commonalities between autism and synesthesia have been noted. They share similar traits such as heightened sensory sensitivity and attention to detail and, like autism, synesthesia is viewed as biological in origin and unlearned.

Why is this of special interest to autism?

Synesthesia has been speculated to be among features of the savant syndrome, which in turn has been seen among some, but not exclusively, persons with autism. The savant syndrome can be described as possessing an exceptional ability in a particular field, for example, extraordinary talents or skills in music, painting, sculpture, and mathematical calculations, memory, in the context of overall handicap, usually intellectual disability.

Does the savant visual artist see not only shapes and colors but also feel tingling in his/her skin? Does the savant pianist hear the music, feel the keys, and enjoy scents as she plays? Does the superbly skilled woodworker who feels the shape and grains of the wood, also hear music and smell aroma in the music? Could this richness wrought by experiencing multiple sensations underlie the savant’s exceptional and unique abilities? And do such synesthetic qualities mark savants?

What might the commonality of synesthesia and its possible link to savant status as well as autism imply for understanding and managing autism? As noted earlier, while a higher than expected prevalence of synesthesia has been seen with autism, those synesthetic traits are present only in a subset (about 20%) of persons with autism. Thus, could the co-presence of synesthesia, with or without savant status, and autism identify a subset of persons on the autism spectrum.

Implications?

If there is such a subset, what other observable or underlying neurobiology traits characterize that subset? How could recognition and comprehension of such a subset impact the management of autistic persons, especially those with extraordinary (aka savant) qualities. Such knowledge and subsequent practice could optimize the positive abilities of this unusual subset of autistic persons who have synesthetic qualities. This would be a welcome departure from one-size-fits-all, under-nourishing formative methods that fail to optimize exceptional talent.

In addition, greater understanding of the behavioral and neurobiological linkages between autism and synesthesia could be productive towards optimizing the positive abilities and traits, not only of a select subtype, but also of others in the larger population of persons with autism.

References:

Van Leeuwen T, Van Lier R. Spectrum/ Autism Research News, June 16, 2020.

Harrison JE & Baron-Cohen S (Eds) 1996. Synesthesia: Classic and contemporary readings. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

%d bloggers like this: